A Study of the Effectiveness of Media Literacy Education

Note: I wrote this as a graduate student in Mass Communication at Texas State University. This paper was accepted for presentation at the school’s annual Mass Communication Week Conference.

Introduction

As American mass communication media become increasingly complex, scholars continue their calls for better attention to media literacy in the nation’s schools. James Potter, for example, points to a story told by Sherwood Schwartz, producer of the television classic Gilligans Island. After the show had aired for its first few weeks in 1964, Schwartz reports, the U.S. Coast Guard began receiving serious complaints from people angry that the agency was unable to rescue the show’s fictional characters who were stranded on a pacific island. (1998)

“Hearing a story like this, we are likely to smile and think that those people must be extremely media illiterate to be so influenced by the media that they could not tell the difference between reality and fantasy. We smugly feel that we don’t have that problem – but remember… we are constantly faced with the challenge of controlling the media’s influence on us, and the difference between us and the viewers who contacted the Coast Guard is only a matter of degree. All of us must continually decide how closely media messages reflect real life. Sometimes these decisions are relatively easy … Other decisions are harder to make accurately.” (Potter, p. 22)

Sissella Bok, carrying forth the same overall theme, points to many studies in which media violence has been found to have at least some negative effect on audiences, particularly children. (1998) Like Potter and dozens (at least) of others, she promotes media literacy education as one way to counteract the negative influences. Media literacy, she says, “views all media as offering scope for participants to learn not to submit passively to whatever comes along, but instead to examine offerings critically.” (p. 141)

The literature of media literacy is, perhaps, overstocked with points such as these. Likewise, there are plenty of suggestions such as those from Hepburn (1999) and Dugald (1999) for ways in which media literacy can be incorporated into all levels of school curriculum. And there are a number of case studies such as that by Manzo (2000) which discuss how media literacy has, indeed, been successful in some American schools.

The success stories are relatively rare, however, and therefore nearly every writer on the topic finds himself, for at least a moment, being frustrated. Media literacy proponents routinely take jabs at an American educational establishment that seems curiously slow in adopting media literacy in its schools. Renowned journalists Walter Cronkite and Hugh Downs have even weighed it, joining dozens of other media experts in bemoaning that “the United States lags behind other countries when it comes to media literacy.” (Magee, 1996) Taken as a whole, media literacy literature seems to scream from the most progressive of mass communication intellectuals “Americans have a problem adequately understanding the media to which they are increasingly being exposed. We know how to fix that problem. Why is it that so few people take us seriously?!”

But in 1999 two of America’s leading media literacy exerts found that media literacy does seem to be taken seriously in American schools — at least on paper. (Kubey & Baker, 1999) In light of a 1998 New York Times report which said that only 12 states at that time had curricular guidelines for media study, the pair took a detailed look at the official curriculum statements in all 50 states. “We have found to our own surprise – and that of all the media educators with whom we’ve spoken – that at least 48 state curricular frameworks now contain one or more elements calling for some form of media education,” they said. (p. 56) In particular, they found that “Texas unquestionably presents the most developed and comprehensive media education framework. Florida’s and North Carolina’s are also impressive. Kansas and Kentucky were the only states we found that did not include nonprint media education, at least in the educational frameworks we were able to locate.” (p. 57)

At first glance, then, the Kubey and Baker study seems to indicate that media literacy proponents have quietly — even unconsciously — won their case. Could it be that media literacy is a significant part of the American education experience after all? This paper investigates that possibility.

The following pages report on a study designed to find out what some Texas college students think and know about some of the media literacy concepts the Kubey and Baker study indicates they should have been exposed to — in one way or another– during their first twelve years of school. Media literacy proponents should not be satisfied with a simple “paper-tiger” showing that schools are teaching media literacy while, in fact, that teaching is largely ineffective. This paper asks if, perhaps, that is just what Kubey and Baker uncovered.

Definition of Media Literacy

Any discussion of media literacy should be careful to establish early on a definition of the term. This is because media literacy appears in literature as being everything from the ability to use oral and written language, to an accumulation of knowledge about pop culture, to a critical cultural issue, to the ability to access various types of media, to suggestions for parents concerned about limiting their child’s access to violence and pornography. (Potter, p. 3) Confusion, then, is the likely result if a paper does not specify exactly what it means by media literacy.

The comments below from television writer Walter Goodman, probably the most outspoken American critic of media literacy, illustrate the problems such confusion can create for the entire media literacy movement. (Magee, 1996) Not knowing what ( to use his sarcastic name) “the media literacy gang” means by the term, Goodman simply dismisses the idea as “another fad” in education — akin to sex education, drivers education and home economics.

“Literacy really only has two meanings. One is an acquaintance with cultural subject of the past — the classics. And the other means knowing how to read and write. You put those two things together for a course that has nothing to do with reading and writing — and has nothing to do with the classics certainly — well, it’s just another way to mess up language for promotional purposes.” (Magee, 1996)

But Goodman’s definition is a far cry from that of the “gang” of educators he derides. Below is a look at what media literacy proponents typically mean by the term.

“Media literacy is education”

That remark from David Considine during a televised town hall meeting about media literacy (Magee, 1996) seems to sums up the experts’ consensus about the definition. Beginning with a 1982 declaration from the United Nation’s Scientific, Educational and Cultural Organization media literacy has been seen, world-wide, as generally aiming to be a “preparation for responsible citizenship.” (Buckingham, 2001) While that goal is sometimes criticized as being too broad, it reflects what American scholars have long said about education in general: “Citizens are expected to make important and independent decisions about governmental problems and their political future. It is clear that many of these decisions require problem solving of a very high order. It is impossible to tell an individual in advance how to vote or even the basis on which he should vote. These are matters he must decide repeatedly throughout his life whenever a major election takes place. But more than specific elections and voting is the concept of individuals in a democracy as independent decisions makers who, in the last analysis, are responsible for the conduct of a democratic political system as well as a democratic way of life.” (Bloom, 1956, p. 41)

The above quotation comes from Taxonomy of Educational Objectives, a classic education text which established the current American educational tradition of classifying learning objectives into six major classes: knowledge, comprehension, application, analysis, synthesis and evaluation. This classification system, known to education scholars as the famous Bloom’s Taxonomy, addresses the broadness of educational goals. It, therefore, is at the heart of our definition of media literacy.

Bloom’s classes are hierarchical; they each (except for knowledge, of course) require that students master objectives in the lower classes before advancing to the next classes. The “high order” problem solving skills discussed in the quotation typically fall in the analysis, synthesis and evaluation classes. Education scholars call these classes “the critical thinking skills”. (Bloom, 1956) And critical thinking skills applied a student’s interaction with mass media (television, newspapers, radio, internet, advertising, etc.) is media literacy in a nutshell, the major American experts agree. (Magee, 1996)

But what, then, is critical thinking?

The careful reader will note that the potential vagueness of “critical thinking” presents yet another problem for media literacy, a field day of sorts for skeptics like Walter Goodman: “Critical viewing skills? This is just jargon from academia. Everyone has critical viewing skills. Are we going to teach people how to have the right critical viewing skills? I’ve been reviewing for quite a few years, and I wouldn’t know how to do that. I don’t know what the right critical viewing skills are, and schools don’t know either.”(Magee, 1996)

While a thorough discussion of the very complex Bloom’s Taxonomy is beyond the scope of this paper, an overview of what the system says about the critical thinking classes is needed to address the critics’ concerns.

“ABOUT ANALYSIS: At a somewhat more advanced level than the skills of comprehension and application are those involved in analysis. In comprehension the emphasis is on the grasp of the meaning and intent of the material. In application it is on remembering and bringing to bear upon given material the appropriate generalizations or principals. Analysis emphasizes the breakdown of the material into its constituent parts and detection of the relationships of the parts and of the way they are organized. It may also be directed at the techniques and devices used to convey the meaning or to establish the conclusion of a communication.” (Bloom, 1956, p. 144)

“ABOUT SYNTHESIS: Synthesis is here defined as the putting together of elements and parts so as to form a whole. This is a process of working with elements, parts, etc., and combining them in such a way as to constitute a pattern or structure not clearly there before. Generally this would involved a recombination of parts of previous experience with new material, reconstructed into a new and more or less well-integrated whole… It should be emphasized that this is not completely free creative expression since generally the student is expected to work within the limits set by particular problems, materials or some theoretical and methodological framework.” (p. 162)

“ABOUT EVALUATION: Evaluation is defined as the making of judgments about the value, for some purpose, of ideas, works, solutions, methods, material, etc. It involves the use of criteria as well as standards for appraising the extent to which particulars are accurate, effective, economical, or satisfying. The judgments may be either quantitative or qualitative, and the criteria may be either those determined by the student or those which are given to him.” (p. 185)

By teaching objectives which fall into these very specific categories (and which, therefore, build upon previously taught objectives which fall into the knowledge, comprehension and application classes) media literacy can give students the intellectual tools they need to adequately analyze, synthesis, and evaluate media messages and, thus, be good participants in society. The development (and teaching) of such specific objectives is a tricky endeavor, to say the least. But, despite the worries of skeptics like Goodman, it is possible — especially after a careful study of Bloom’s Taxonomy which, for decades, has been required reading for new teachers in America.

With media literacy now defined, as educators understand it, we are ready to turn to our study, which reviews and then tests some of the media literacy objectives listed in the Kubey and Baker report. In the study we examine the objectives in terms of Bloom’s Taxonomy, judging how well they contribute to media literacy’s “critical thinking” goal. Then we analyze the results of our survey which was designed to find out whether a group of Texas college students learned those objectives effectively in their pre-college days.

The study

In late 2001 a 10-question “Media Literacy Survey” was given to 248 students at Southwest Texas State University in San Marcos, Texas. The survey tested what the students know and think about the three media literacy objectives identified most often in Kubey and Baker’s report. Each student was in the university’s “College Writing” class taught by English Department faculty and required of all undergraduate students in the university. Most of these students (222 of them to be precise) were college freshmen who graduated from high school in the spring of 2001. The students were given the survey by either their course instructor or by this paper’s author during one class period sometime between November 22 and December 3, 2001. They were asked to complete the survey on a voluntary and anonymous basis. A list of the questions is in figure 1. (It might be important to note that this survey was also given to 162 students in advanced mass communication courses at the same university. The intent was to compare the results of the two groups to test the hypothesis that students formally trained in college mass communication are likely to be more media literate than those who have not. That hypothesis received support in our results, but that is beyond the scope of the present paper.)

Fig. 1

Survey Questions

1. For practical reasons, reporters can’t always be involved with the groups which they cover (because they generally cover a great many different groups in the course of their careers). In an ideal world, however, it would be best to have news articles written mostly by members of the groups involved in the stories.

Agree _____ No opinion _____ Disagree _____

2. Heavy viewers of television shows involving crime are likely to characterize the world as a dangerous place.

Agree _____ No opinion _____ Disagree _____

3. People tend to base their political opinions more on what their friends and family members think than on what media personalities such as Rush Limbaugh think.

Agree _____ No opinion _____ Disagree _____

(More questions on the back)

4. If the U.S. President makes a persuasive argument on national television, the majority of Americans will likely change their behavior as a result.

Agree _____ No opinion _____ Disagree _____

5. The media tend to do an effective job of telling their audiences what to think.

Agree _____ No opinion _____ Disagree _____

6. The opinions of callers to radio talk shows can be a good gauge of U.S. public opinion.

Agree _____ No opinion _____ Disagree _____

7. One television station’s story about a house fire quotes only a neighbor who watched the event from start to finish from her living room window. Another station’s story about the same fire quotes only the city’s fire chief. Which story is likely better?

The story quoting the neighbor ______

The story quoting the fire chief ______

Don’t have an opinion ______

8. Which of the following sentences represents the better writing?

______ The guard said that two residents of the correctional facility expired.

_____ The guard said that two prisoners died.

_____ No opinion

9. Do you think the following sentence has unwarranted elements of a stereotype?

Kay Bailey Hutchison, who was once a cheerleader for the University of Texas, has built a strong career as a politician.
Yes ____No _____No opinion _____

10. In an effort to make more money through advertising sales, a newspaper begins selling sponsorships of individual stories. The phrase “This article is a service of (company name)” now appears at the top of each story, underneath the by-line. Advertisers love this new approach, and profits for the paper are at an all-time high. Some reporters have expressed concern about this practice, but the newspaper’s managers have said they plan to continue selling the sponsorships. Please check the statement which would best represent your thoughts about this issue.

_____ The managers owe it to the company to bring in as much money as possible. The practice should continue.

_____ The reporters are right, the practice needs to stop.

_____ I don’t care whether the practice continues or not.

Creating the survey

Categorizing the objectives: Kubey and Baker list 458 media literacy objectives in the nation’s curriculum. Because of that volume, the first step in creating this study’s student survey was to categorize each of the objectives. The survey would then test the most popular categories.

The objectives could be divided into 14 categories, and each objective’s category number appears in the appendix. For each category this study’s author wrote an objective which best represents all of the Kubey and Baker objectives in its class. That list of representative objectives appears in Fig. 2. In parenthesis is the percentage of the Kubey and Baker objectives that fit into each class. The objectives that can be said to be in the Bloom’s Taxonomy’s critical thinking skills classifications are highlighted in bold.

Interestingly, more than 70 percent of Kubey and Baker’s objectives fall into the three most popular classes. So, those are the objectives which the survey tests. They are category 2. The student will apply critical thinking to media content ; category 4. The student will analyze the indirect messages in media content (27.95 percent); and 5. The student will consider the media’s influence. The survey asks for students’ opinions on three questions related to category 2, three related to category 4, and four related to category 5. The other categories were not tested in this survey.

Fig. 2

Objective categories
(bold indicates a critical thinking skill)

1. (1.9%) – The student will exhibit responsibility in media selection.
2. (17.03%) – The student will apply critical thinking to media content.
3. (9.83%) – The student will become an effective communicator.
4. (27.95%) – The student will analyze indirect messages in media content.

5. (25.76%) – The student will consider the media’s influence.
6. (1.97%) – The student will analyze events discussed in media coverage.
7. (3.93%) – The student will put media information to practical use.
8. (6.33%) – The student will understand information gathering techniques.
9. (1.31%) – The student will consider how the media reflects society.
10. (2.62%) – The student will be exposed to media material from a variety of cultural perspectives.
11. (.66%) – The student will understand how the media contributes to democracy.
12. (.22%) – The student will apply media information to personal experience.
13. (.22%) – The student will identify various forms of mass communication.
14. (.22%) – The student will explain how events described in the media can be interpreted differently by different individuals.

Analyzing the objectives: As Fig. 2 shows, two of the three objectives tested on the survey are a critical thinking skills as defined by Bloom’s Taxonomy, but, of those, only Objective 4 is useful. Objective 2 is, almost comically, too broad to be of much use to a teacher trying to design a lesson. We address the problems with Objective 2 in the discussion and results section of the paper. For present purposes it will suffice to say that we tested that objective — to the extent that it was possible to test — because, despite its vagueness, it is the third most popular of the objective categories. The third objective tested , Objective 5, is not a critical thinking skill. That objective would fall into the knowledge class of the Blooms Taxonomy.

Writing the questions

Each of the questions on the survey asks for student opinions about media literacy topics. In each case, students were given an opportunity to register a “correct” opinion – hereafter referred to as the media literate response — that is based on media research and concepts commonly taught in university journalism courses and upheld by professional journalists. Below is a description of how each question applies to the concept it is designed to test and how the “media literate” response is backed by media research and/or standard journalistic thought.

Analyzing Indirect Messages:

Questions 1, 9, and 10 test category 4 above, “The student will analyze indirect messages in media content.” In general, these questions attempt to discover if respondents can recognize instances of the indirect messages Yopp and McAdams (1999) and other textbook authors say good mass communicators should avoid sending through their media. Examples are bias and stereotypes. Two of the questions also attempt to discover if, once the indirect messages are recognized, the respondents are concerned about them.

Question 1 asks respondents to decide if, in an ideal world, it would be best to have news articles written mostly by members of the groups involved in the stories. Article III of the American Society of Newspaper Editors addresses this issue as it is addressed in most basic American journalism courses: “Independence. Journalists must avoid impropriety and the appearance of impropriety as well as any conflict of interest or the appearance of conflict. They should neither accept anything nor pursue any activity that might compromise or seem to compromise their integrity.” (“ASNE’s Statement of principals” 1975)

The media literate response to question 1, therefore, would be to disagree with the statements that it would be best of have news articles written mostly by members of the groups involved in the news.

Question 9 asks respondents if the following sentence has unwarranted elements of a stereotype: “Kay Bailey Hutchison, who was once a cheerleader for the University of Texas, has built a strong career as a politician.” Yopp and McAdams (1999) are not the only writers to point out that mass communicators occasionally “thoughtlessly use language that treats women as inferior or that is demeaning or insulting” (p. 190). They cite a recent example: “Consider media coverage of repeated allegations against President Clinton about his sexual conduct. One of the female protagonists, Linda Tripp, who taped Monica Lewinsky, was described in a Washington Post article as a “divorced middle-aged mother who needed to keep the paychecks coming…” The implication by the writer was that Tripp was a woman who would do anything for money. Readers questioned the relevance of labeling her as divorced and middle-aged. The facts would have been better placed in a paragraph devoted to biographical information about Tripp.” (p. 191)

The same conclusion could be drawn in the above sentence about Kay Bailey Hutchison. Therefore, the media literate response to question 9 would be to conclude that the sentence does have unwarranted elements of a stereotype.

Question 10 presents a fictitious scenario in which a newspaper has begun the practice of selling sponsorships of individual stories. The phrase “This article is a service of (company name)” now appears at the top of each story underneath the by-line. The survey asks respondents whether this practice should continue. Article III of the ASNE’s statement of principals (quoted above) applies to this scenario, as does Article II below: “Article II — Freedom of the Press. Freedom of the press belongs to the people. It must be defended against encroachment or assault from any quarter, public or private. Journalists must be constantly alert to see that the public’s business is conducted in public. They must be vigilant against all who would exploit the press for selfish purposes.” (“ASNE’s Statement of principals” 1975)

The media literate response to question 10, therefore, would be to conclude that the practice should stop.

Considering Media’s Influence:

Questions 2-5 test category 5 above, “The student will consider the media’s influence.” They attempt to discover if respondents have a good practical understanding of some of the more important discoveries media researchers have made about the media’s power.

Question 2 asks respondents if they think heavy viewers of television shows involving crime are likely to characterize the world as a dangerous place. This question involves the famous cultivation theory developed by Gerbner and his colleagues at the University of Pennsylvania (1980). Gerbner et al. found in their research that heavy television watching makes people feel that the world is an unsafe place. The media literate response to question 2, therefore, would be to conclude that heavy viewers of television shows involving crime are, indeed, more likely to characterize the world as a dangerous place.

Question 3 ask respondents if they think people tend to base their political opinions more on what friends and family members think than on what media personalities such as Rush Limbaugh think. Lazarsfeld et al (1944) addressed this issue in their famous book which established the two-step flow theory of mass communication. In a nutshell, this theory says that people tend to base their political opinions more on what “opinion leaders” whom they know think than on what’s suggested to them via the mass media. Based on this theory, then, the media literate response to question 3 would be to agree with the statement that people tend to base their political opinions more on what friends and family members think than on what media personalities think.

Question 4 asks respondents if they think that the majority of Americans will likely change their behavior after the U.S. President makes a persuasive argument on national television. Early mass communication researchers, busily studying propaganda and believing in the “magic bullet theory” of mass communication, might have guessed that Americans will change their behavior after a president’s nationally televised persuasive argument. But the accidental discovery of the two step flow theory (Lazarsfeld et al) changed that in 1944. Researchers now know that the effects of mass media messages are rarely as direct, uniform, and immediate as the magic bullet theory would suggest. The media literate response to question 4, therefore, would be to disagree with the statement which statement that says most Americans will likely change their behavior as a result of the U.S. President’s persuasive argument on national television.

Question 5 asks respondents if they think media tend to do an effective job of telling their audiences what to think. McCombs and Shaw (1972) found in their famous study of voters in Chapel Hill South Carolina that there was a strong relationship between the news covered in the media and the news voters considered important. Thus, agenda-setting theory was born, based on the idea that media can do a good job of telling audiences what to think about, but not necessarily what to think. The media literate response to question 5, then, would be to disagree with the statement that media tend to do an effective job of telling their audiences what to think.

Thinking Critically About Media:

Questions 6 – 8 test category 2, “The student will apply critical thinking to media content.” They attempt to discover if the respondents can apply critical thinking skills to their consumption of mass media products. By critical thinking, we mean the ability to use reason to evaluate a mass communication message’s effectiveness and accuracy.

Question 6 asks respondents if they think that callers to radio talk shows can be a good gauge of U.S. public opinion. Textbooks covering social science topics — such as Singletary (1994)–typically are very clear that scientists rely on carefully selected random samples of specific populations when measuring public opinion. Media literate people, therefore, would understand, then, that since radio talk shows callers are not part of a carefully selected random sample of a population, their opinions are not a good gauge of public opinion. The media literate response to question 6 would be to disagree with the statement “The opinions of callers to radio talk shows can be a good gauge of U.S. public opinion.”

Question 7 asks respondents to decide if, in a television story about a house fire, a neighbor who watched the event from start to finish or the city’s fire chief would be the better source. Textbooks designed for beginning journalists – such as Stovall (2002) – typically point out that “reliance on officials sources” (Stovall 2002 p. 70) is an important journalistic consideration. Official sources, who have the knowledge and background to provide accurate information, are typically preferred over sources that can offer simply speculation and opinion. (p. 70) The media literate response to question 7, therefore, would be to conclude that the fire chief would be the better source.

Question 8 asks respondents to decide which of the following sentences represents the better writing: “The guard said that two residents of the correctional facility expired.” Or “The guard said that two prisoners died.” Both sentences are based on an example in Yopp and McAdams’s (1999) textbook for beginning media writers. Yopp and McAdams conclude, as should a media literate reader, that the second sentence is better because it’s shorter, easier to understand, and more clearly communicates that the two “residents” are actually “prisoners”. (p. 151) The media literate response to question 7, therefore, would be to conclude that the second sentence represents the better writing.

Figure 3 lists the media literate response as well as a brief summary of supporting materials for each question.

Fig. 3

Survey Questions

1. For practical reasons, reporters can’t always be involved with the groups which they cover (because they generally cover a great many different groups in the course of their careers). In an ideal world, however, it would be best to have news articles written mostly by members of the groups involved in the stories.

Media Literate Response: Disagree
Supporting Material: ASNE Principals

2. Heavy viewers of television shows involving crime are likely to characterize the world as a dangerous place.

Media Literate Response: Agree
Supporting Material: Cultivation Theory

3. People tend to base their political opinions more on what their friends and family members think than on what media personalities such as Rush Limbaugh think.

Media Literate Response: Agree
Supporting Material: Opinion Leader Research

4. If the U.S. President makes a persuasive argument on national television, the majority of Americans will likely change their behavior as a result.

Media Literate Response: Disagree
Suyporting Material: Two Step Flow Theory

5. The media tend to do an effective job of telling their audiences what to think.

Media Literate Response: Disagree
Supporting Material: Agenda Setting Research

6. The opinions of callers to radio talk shows can be a good gauge of U.S. public opinion.

Media Literate Response: Disagree
Supporting Material: Scientific Method

7. One television station’s story about a house fire quotes only a neighbor who watched the event from start to finish from her living room window. Another station’s story about the same fire quotes only the city’s fire chief. Which story is likely better?

Media Literate Response: Story quoting the fire chief
Supporting Material: Journalism’s “reliance on official sources” rule

8. Which of the following sentences represents the better writing?

______ The guard said that two residents of the correctional facility expired.

_____ The guard said that two prisoners died.

_____ No opinion

Media Literate Response: Sentence Two
Supporting Material: writing textbooks

9. Do you think the following sentence has unwarranted elements of a stereotype?

Kay Bailey Hutchison, who was once a cheerleader for the University of Texas, has built a strong career as a politician.

Media Literate Response: Yes
Supporting Material: writing textbooks

10. In an effort to make more money through advertising sales, a newspaper begins selling sponsorships of individual stories. The phrase “This article is a service of (company name)” now appears at the top of each story, underneath the by-line. Advertisers love this new approach, and profits for the paper are at an all-time high. Some reporters have expressed concern about this practice, but the newspaper’s managers have said they plan to continue selling the sponsorships. Please check the statement which would best represent your thoughts about this issue.

Media Literate Response: The Practice Should Stop
Supporting Material: ASNE Principals

The results

The students’ answers to the questions were analyzed In regards to the two previously mentioned hypothesis. Hypothesis 1 guessed that more than half of high school graduates lack a solid understanding of most of the media literacy concepts Kubey and Baker found listed in the states’ curriculum guidelines. Hypothesis 2 guessed that students who are directly exposed to such concepts in formal college courses in mass communication (i.e. journalism, advertising, broadcasting, public relations, etc.) are more likely to have a solid understanding of media literacy concepts than those who have not. Both hypotheses found support in the results.

How the English students did

In regards to Hypothesis 1, the responses from the 248 freshman English students were examined. More than half of the students chose the media literate response on only 3 of the 10 questions. The hypothesis, therefore, is supported: on 70 percent of the questions, more than half of the students showed a lack of solid understanding of media literacy concepts. Table 1 lists the percentage of media literate responses for each question.
Table 1

Question
Number
Number of
Media Literate Responses
Media Literate
Percentage
1 101 40.7%
2 105 42.3%
3 175 70.6%
4 105 42.3%
5 45 18.1%
6 144 46%
7 56 22.6%
8 134 54%
9 145 58.5%
10 56 22.6%

As Table 1 shows Hypothesis I does not find support in the answers to questions 3, 8 and 9, but, taken on a whole, the survey answers are evidence that more than half of high school graduates lack a solid understanding of the media literacy concepts tested. This evidence comes despite the fact that 90 percent of the students surveyed reported that at least some of their previous education was in Texas, one of the most comprehensive media literacy guidelines in the nation. (Kubey & Baker, 1999) Media literacy educators will also perhaps find the responses to questions 5, 7 and 10 particularly troubling. In each of those cases, less than a quarter of the respondents chose the media literate response.

Comparing the English to the Mass Communication students

In regards to Hypothesis 2, the survey answers from the 248 English students were compared statistically to those from the 162 mass communication students. On six of the questions, the groups showed a statistically significant difference, and, on all six, the mass communication students were more likely to select the media literate response. This hypothesis, therefore, also finds support.

Table’s 2 and 3 show the results of the comparison of each of the questions which showed a significant difference. To prepare for a t-test for independent samples, each answer to each question was coded in the data as either 1,2 or 3 (depending on its order of appearance after the question). The means of the answers of the two were then compared and tested for significance.

Table 2

Comparison of Results
(only statistically significant results are listed)

Question
Number
Value of
Media LiterateResponse
English Mean Mass Comm.Mean Group Closest
to MediaLiterate Value
1 3 2.03 2.38 Mass Comm.
2 1 2.0 1.67 Mass Comm.
6 3 2.08 2.27 Mass Comm.
7 2 1.45 1.66 Mass Comm.
9 1 1.54 1.33 Mass Comm.
10 2 2.41 2.1 Mass Comm.

Table 3

Answers by Mass Communication Students
(Statistically significant differences in bold)

QuestionNumber Number ofMedia LiterateResponses Media LiteratePercentage English Students’Media Literate Pct.
1 104 64.2% 40.7%
2 96 59.3% 42.3%
3 122 75.3% 70.6%
4 64 39.5% 42.3%
5 41 25.3% 18.1%
6 95 58.6% 46%
7 66 40.7% 22.6%
8 134 82.7% 54.%
9 118 72.8% 58.5%
10 78 48.1% 22.6%

While a statistically significant comparison cannot be made of the answers to question number 4, it its interesting to note in table 3 that the mass communication students had a lower percentage of media literate answers on than question than did the English students. Absent that bit of statistically insignificant evidence, Hypothesis 2 receives a great deal of support from our comparison. It seems clear in this analysis that the students who were directly exposed to basic media literacy concepts in formal courses in mass communication (i.e. journalism, advertising, broadcasting, public relations, etc.) were more likely to have a solid understanding of those concepts.

Some will likely suggest that our conclusion in regards to this hypothesis is too much of the “common sense” variety to be of much use. And that criticism would probably have merit. A closer look at the mass communication students’ answers, however, gives us something which is likely more useful — or perhaps worrisome. Table 3 shows that a majority of the mass communication students did not select the media literate answer on 4 of the 10 questions. (And of particular note should be question number 5, on which just over a quarter of the students chose the media literate answer.) So, the mass communication students, to be sure, proved more media literate than the English students ( most of who, as noted previously, did not choose the media literate answer on 7 of the questions), but, given that the mass communication students have more education — and, specifically, media education — it would seem reasonable to expect them to exhibit even higher levels of media literacy than these results indicate. Admittedly, it would be difficult to establish a universally accepted “improvement threshold” by which to judge the mass communication students. So, for present purposes, then, it will have to suffice to say that Hypothesis 2 is supported, but to a debatable degree.

Discussion

The limited scope of this study, of course, prohibits any sweeping conclusions about the overall effectiveness of media literacy education in the United States. But the study does point to the possibility that, while the literature is practically overwhelmed with calls for more media literacy education, what may be more important is better education. The media literacy field would benefit from more (and larger scale) studies such as this one which test what’s currently being taught.

A Paper Tiger?

Looking at Kubey & Baker’s review of the United State’s curriculum guidelines, one could be forgiven for mistaking the United States as one of the world’s leaders in media literacy. On paper, it seems, nearly every state takes the subject seriously, nicely incorporating it into several key areas of the curriculum and, in some cases, even offering stand-alone “Media Literacy” courses. But this study is evidence that, even in a state which seems to take media literacy more seriously than most, there is a big difference between what United States educators say about media literacy and what they do about it. If Texas were as committed to media literacy in its schools as it curriculum guidelines suggest, it would seem likely, to name just one example from our results, that more than 18 percent of the English students in this paper’s survey would know that the media, in fact, have been proven rather ineffective at telling their audiences what to think (see question 5 in the survey above).

A case can be made that educators in the United States have included the media literacy concepts so thoroughly in their curriculum simply as an appeasement. The thinking could be that, so long as media literacy concepts are officially listed in the curriculum guidelines, the cries for more media literacy are addressed and, therefore, will eventually end. Media literacy experts should not settle for that, of course. More studies such as this one will help force educators to continue to do more than just talk about teaching media literacy.

Colleges should take note

University faculty and administrators would do well to be at least mildly concerned about the survey answers from mass communication students. While it is true that, as our hypothesis guessed, the mass communication students proved more media literate than the English students, the survey asked about very basic concepts which are presented again and again throughout any mass communication undergraduate’s academic career. It is troubling to this author, at least, that the mass communication students did not exhibit media literacy levels at something approaching 100 percent. (So as not to necessarily indict Southwest Texas State University’s Mass Communication Department on, it should be noted that this study’s survey was pre-tested on more than two dozen people. At least half of these people were mass communication or journalism graduates from other universities, and they failed to select the media literate answer on anywhere from 3 to 8 questions. The only pre-testers to select all 10 media literate answers were Southwest Texas State mass communication faculty members.)

While it would be encouraging for more universities to follow the example of Alverno College in Wisconsin (Wulff, 1997) and firmly establish media literacy goals throughout their curriculum, this study points to what may be an even more pressing need. Colleges should examine just how media literate their own mass communication students are. Perhaps, upon discovering that its mass communication students’ media literacy levels are lower than would be expected, a college could adjust its mass communication curriculum accordingly. That adjustment could then become the pilot for an eventual campus-wide media literacy program.

Conclusion

Those who chuckle at Sherwood Schwartz’s story about the naive viewers of Gilligan’s Island will also likely marvel at today’s Phil Hendrie radio show. This program makes its living, more or less, off of a lack of media literacy in the United States. Each day on that show Hendrie interviews fictitious characters with preposterous ideas about society, and each day, without fail, Hendrie airs calls from angry listeners who do not realize the character is made-up. This makes for very entertaining radio, but it also says something alarming about American society. Anecdotal evidence such as this seems to suggest that Americans, on a whole, lack much in terms of basic media literacy skills. So, there is a need for more scientific study in this area, especially as media literacy proponents continue their decades-old calls for more media literacy programs in schools.

This paper reports on the results of one such (albeit small-scale) study which, it is hoped, will inspire others. This study cannot translate into generalizations about the United States public, but the results should not be encouraging to those who hope that Phil Hendrie’s daily callers represent only the tiniest, most uneducated, portion of Americans.

The study found that more than half of the freshman English students surveyed lacked a solid understanding of media literacy concepts to which they had, reportedly, been exposed in high school. Further, it found that, while mass communication students showed a better understanding of media literacy concepts, it’s debatable whether their level of improvement, after a considerably more exposure to media literacy concepts, is sufficient.

The literature of media literacy is filled with suggestions — and pleas — for implementing media education in United States schools. These calls have not fallen entirely on deaf ears. In many schools, media literacy initiatives are in place, and, as Kubey and Baker show, nearly every state in America has some provision for media literacy in its curriculum. So, while the continued maneuvering for more media literacy in American schools still, certainly, has its place, experts should also now consider seriously evaluating the effectiveness of the media literacy that is currently being taught.